Donnerstag, 12. Juli 2012

How Do We Link Words to Their Meanings?



 “Exceptions confirm the rule”, this proverb could be applied to a family that suffers from a unique memory deficit: at least four generations struggle to assign words to concepts and meaning, which results in problems to memorize word lists or even stories they were just told. Although the family in question seems to be the only one with this specific trait, research could bring results that explain language development in general. Tests have shown, that members of this family have verbal intelligence like everybody else and non-verbal intelligence that is even above average, which contradicts some of the family member’s self-perception of being stupid. What makes their brains different from the ones of the control group is the comparatively little amount of “neurons and glial cells” – the so-called “grey matter” in a brain area that is “linked to processing word meaning”. This abnormality might be caused by one single gene, which is currently searched for by geneticist Simon Fisher and psychiatrist David Skuse. Yet, although these findings could help us to better understand our brains, it probably comes a little late for the family.

-Malin

http://www.nature.com/news/gene-mutation-sought-to-explain-mysterious-language-problem-1.10851

The Weight Problem



Overweight and obesity might not only be individual problems anymore but also collective ones. Biologists have calculated the human biomass as an additional means to measure the impact – and in this case the word “impact” can be taken quite literally – of the human race on our planet and have found out, that out of the total human biomass of 287 million tonnes, 15 million tonnes can be traced back to overweight and 3.5 million tonnes to obesity. This is quite a lot, especially considering the fact that this additional mass of 15 million tonnes is equivalent to 242 million humans with an average weight of 62 kg.
That sheds light not only on the problem of “population growth” but also on the problem of “overconsumption” as epidemiologist Ian Robert states.

Yet, what might or should be the consequences of this finding? The more someone weighs, the more energy input his body needs. The more energy is needed, the more the planet is exploited. When following this argument, obviously human weight should be reduced in order to save the planet. Yet, implementing policies that help solving this problem can lead to absurd and problematic decisions like the one-child-policy in China. In addition, overweight and obesity have different causes in the range from overeating to genetic defects, which makes it difficult to implement any law that would regulate the weight of each individual. Only the diet industry can rejoice: now they have one argument more to convince people to buy their products. The possible slogan could be: save the planet – drink (insert name of protein drink here)!
-Malin












No More Bites



What predators do we have? It seems, that we as human race got rid of all animals that could possibly do us harm: we either hunted them to death or put them behind bars. The few accidents like crocodile or bear attacks that happen occasionally don’t threaten us on a big scale. Yet, there is a species – no, even worse: 3500 species – that is able to kill millions of people with just a sting: the mosquito. That answers the question from the beginning: we do have a predator, but it is tiny.

Nature, an international science journal, put out the questions to scientists working in the field: “So what would happen if there were none? Would anyone or anything miss them?” Or differently put: would any ecosystem become unstable because one link in the food chain was missing? What sounds like pure curiosity in the beginning can easily turn into an ethical matter, because the suggestion behind the question points at an eradication of 3500 species in favor of one. The answers from the specialists point in different directions and reach from concerns about the ecosystem in the Arctic tundra to utterances such as: “[the] ecological effect of eliminating harmful mosquitoes is that you have more people. That’s the consequence.” What Daniel Strickman from the US Department of Agriculture in Beltsville, Maryland points out here could be turned around in asking: what harm would humans do to our ecosystem if they lost their main predator?

This question, however, doesn’t appear to feel itchy to any of the scientists mentioned in the article – on the contrary. To seek a way of getting rid of an annoying discomfort seems to be the common viewpoint, which the writer of the article, Janet Fang, puts in a nutshell by stating that “[it] is the limitations of mosquito-killing methods, not the limitations of intent, that make a world without mosquitoes unlikely.”


-Malin

Can Chimpanzees See Numbers in Colors?



Although we as human beings tend to preen ourselves on our perceived superior memory ability in comparison to ‘other’ animals, test results gained at Kyoto University’s Primate Research Institute suggest otherwise. Chimpanzees appear to be far better in memorizing than any human when it comes to numbers. Tetsuro Matsuzawa, professor of language and intelligence at the aforementioned institute, has conducted several studies with chimpanzees, in which he let them order numbers in consecutive progression from one to nine. These numbers appeared on a computer touch screen for only split seconds, leaving white squares, which the chimpanzees were supposed to touch in the right succession. Especially Ayumu, a chimpanzee that gained stardom through the studies, was able to fulfill the task with an amazing reliability. So far, no human was able to surpass Ayumu in his performance.

Matsuzawa sees the reason for Ayumu’s success in the chimpanzee’s good memory skills – a view that is challenged by psychologist Nicholas Humphrey who suggests a different explanation: Ayumu could experience what is called “synesthesia”, the ability to see numbers in colors. This way, the after-effect of the ‘colored’ numbers could show on the white squares, which then would make it easy for the chimpanzee to tap on the ‘colors’ in a learned order. So far, this ability has been only linked to humans due to their capacity of building chains of numbers or letters, but why should it not also apply to a chimpanzee that has learned how to count?

To date, however, Matsuzawa hasn’t shown any interest in testing this possibility, although the test Humphrey proposes sounds simple: color the squares to begin with and then observe, if the chimpanzee scores the same test results as he usually does with black numbers on white squares.



The Mystery of Self


Argentine writer Jorge Luis Borges introduces in his essay “Borges and I” his sense of self, which oscillates between two seemingly contradictory poles: the “I”, whom Rhodes interprets as the “private” one, and “Borges”, the “public” figure. Whereas these two people couldn’t be more apart from each other in Borges view, they still need each other to not only be able to produce, but also to share their work. In fact, these ‘selves’ are one person.

In her article “Ignorance and Mystery”, structural biologist Gale Rhodes states, that science has the function to tell ignorance apart from mystery with the latter, in contrary to the former, eluding scientific description. She exemplifies the difference between the two by describing the notion of “feeling” in scientific terms, which in her opinion falls short of also giving an explanation of the self that is feeling these sensations in the first place. Drawing on Borges’ idea of two selves, Rhodes assigns the scientist to one and the artist to the other self, claiming, that only the clearance of ignorance in combination with acknowledging the essential mystery of life can constitute a complete human being.

Rhodes displays her idea of science versus art assuming that both are contradictory to each other as well as complementing each other. But what if you follow the scientific path and come to the conclusion, that there really is no such thing as a “self”? What kind of sentimentality makes her think, that there has to be more to a human being, something “mysterious” even?
Although I like her humble approach to science as not the only way to explaining life, I think, reducing the arts to the sole act of experiencing and communicating feeling doesn’t suffice. Either it is necessary to change the paradigms of science or the meaning of art to come to a conclusion that is consistent in itself. What she essentially does, is explaining religion, because religion works the same way: what we cannot explain, we have to believe. Yet, this belief is always dependent on how far we are willing to investigate life, which can mean that we have to take the risk of disenchanting it. But is that so horrible? Neither the “self” nor “feelings” are fixed entities – they are constantly changing. Maybe Rhodes is afraid of losing the romantic in her life and therefore tries to protect it by creating a somewhat ‘holy’ category that is impossible to attack. Acknowledging, however, that there is no “self” does not have to be contradictory to the experience of love and happiness.


-Malin

Mittwoch, 4. Juli 2012

The ‘New Amadeus’: Computer Composing Classical Music That 'Rocks'



Photograph: Gautier Deblonde[1]

Often have I heard of modernist music being subject to a controversial debate, but never before have I encountered a computer – Iamus, ‘named after the son of Apollo who could understand the language of birds’, - being responsible for music critics racking their heads. Iamus’ composition ’Transits – Into an Abyss’, created in the modernist style of Bartók, Ligeti, and Penderecki will be released in September, while the devisor of the computer program, Francisco Vico from Malaga University, is lucky to report that among the performers will be high-class musicians such as the London Symphony Orchestra. He promises that ‘it’s going to be disturbing’, that is for sure.
Question is and remains, how can artificial intelligence be distinguished from human intelligence? How does Iamus actually proceed and can his compositions be considered ‘good’, maybe even ‘better than human’? Where are the differences and can they be perceived? Readers are invited to take a test and judge for themselves on http://www.guardian.co.uk/music/2012/jul/01/iamus-computer-composes-classical-music where they are offered five examples of which four pieces are composed by humans while Iamus wrote exactly one. I can tell, because I took the test, that it is recognizable, indeed, there is something that cannot be named that distinguishes the computer example from Mahler, Ligeti, Penderecki and Strockhausen. Fortunately.
Now, how does Iamus work? He ‘composes by mutating simple starting material in a manner analogous to biological evolution, the compositions each have a musical core, a "genome", that gradually becomes more complex’, it says in the corresponding article. Iamus does so automatically and exclusively do the researchers intervene by specifying the rough length of the piece and the set of instruments. This idea works for all instruments, all genomes, and even for producing variations of popular pieces. Composer Gustavo Díaz-Jerez explains that this single genome can generate a great number of melodies, similar to western music. ‘That’s why the music makes sense’.
Using computers for music compositions is not new. There have already been algorithms like GenJam, GenBebop, and the Continuator that knew how to improvise in the style of Charlie Parker-which is admirable since many humans would not get the gist of  it-but with Iamus it ‘seems to be the first time music composed by computer has been deemed good enough for top-class performers to play’, author Phillip Ball reports. While the LSO was skeptical about the music at first, the quality and expressiveness of the lines Iamus composed were convincing. Díaz-Jerez even pronounces some of Iamus’ works ‘better than those produced by some avant-garde composers’. Whatever our opinion might be, I think we could agree on that at least.                                                                                                                                   Nina